Loughborough University
Browse
Wilcockson_Corrigendum_Wilcockson_Osborne_Ellis.pdf (117.38 kB)

Corrigendum to ‘Digital detox: The effect of smartphone abstinence on mood, anxiety, and craving’ [Add. Behav. 99 (2019) 106013]

Download (117.38 kB)
journal contribution
posted on 2020-11-16, 11:27 authored by Thom WilcocksonThom Wilcockson, AM Osborne, DA Ellis
© 2019 Elsevier Ltd Following publication, the authors became aware of an error that occurred during data entry. Specifically, two columns of data were mislabelled whereby participants craving scores at time 1 were incorrectly labelled as Smartphone Addiction Inventory Scores (SPAI) (and vice versa). Corrected results and figures are provided below. A revised post-print that incorporates all these edits is also available https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/c85kx. Following these changes, the overall results and conclusions remain unchanged. The authors would like to apologise for any inconvenience caused. Introduction ‘The aim of this project is therefore to examine the effect on mood, anxiety, and craving change when participants stop using their smartphone for 24-hours.’ Should read: ‘The aim of this project is therefore to consider how mood, anxiety, and craving change when participants stop using their smartphone for 24-hours.’ Procedure ‘After the abstinence task was completed, participants were asked to return to the lab a fourth and final to complete the selected questionnaires.’ Should read: ‘After the abstinence task was completed, participants were asked to return to the lab a fourth and final time to complete the selected questionnaires.’ Fig. 1 Results Section 4.1 (Mood and Anxiety) should read: ‘Overall, Fig. 2 suggests that mood was lower immediately before the abstinence task, but gradually increased towards the end of the study. A small reduction in anxiety is also apparent during the final session. However, ANOVAs did not reveal a significant main effect of session on mood [F(3,105) = 1.79; p =.15; BF10 = 0.29] or anxiety [F (3,105) = 1.08; p =.36; BF10 = 0.13].’ Section 4.2 (Craving) should read: ‘A significant main effect of session was observed on craving [F (3,105) = 14.15; p <.0005; BF10 > 100]. Uncorrected comparisons revealed that most sessions differed significantly from each other: session 1 [M = 93.95; SD = 45.58] and session 4 [M = 66.86; SD = 37.06; t(35) = 4.67; p <.0005; BF10 > 100]; session 2 [M = 84.80; SD = 45.22 and session 3 [M = 98.78; SD = 44.21; t(40) = 3.09; p =.004; BF10 = 9.68]; session 2 and session 4 [t(35) = 3.93; p <.0005; BF10 = 74.19]; session 3 and session 4 [t(35) = 8.16; p <.0005; BF10 > 100]. However no significant differences were observed between session 1 and session 2 [t(40) = 1.97; p =.06; BF10 > 0.97] or session 1 and session 3 [t(40) = 0.91; p =.38; BF10 > 0.25].’ Section 4.3 (Problematic smartphone usage) should read: ‘During the first session, participants completed the problematic phone usage questionnaire (SPAI). We observed that this measure positively correlated with craving measures taken during session 1 [r (43) = 0.69; p <.0005; BF10 > 100], session 2 [r(39) = 0.60; p <.0005; BF10 > 100], session 3 [r(39) = 0.62; p <.001; BF10 > 100], and session 4 [r(34) = 0.66; p <.0005; BF10 > 100]. Therefore, while levels of craving varied between each session, it would appear that participants who believed they used their smartphone more also reported higher levels of craving. Mood and anxiety scores were not associated with the SPAI at any time point [all p's > 0.1].’ Discussion The first section of the discussion should read: ‘Whether or not behavioural addictions are akin to substance addictions remains a matter of considerable debate (Kardefelt-Winther et al., 2017). However, our results suggest that while smartphone abstinence can lead to craving, mood and anxiety remain unaffected. We also note that while craving did increase during the abstinence period (between sessions 2 and 3), it remained similar to levels reported a week prior to taking part (session 1). Nevertheless, while these results may indicate that smartphone users like to use their smartphones and crave them when they are unavailable, the lack of evidence for mood modification and increased anxiety suggests a key distinction between technology-related behaviours and substance abusers.’ This continues without any further correction from the sentence beginning ‘Substance abusers…’ A sentence in the second paragraph of the discussion: ‘However, while this may provide some evidence to confirm mood modification, our Bayes result suggests that more evidence is required to support any effect of mood before or after any period of smartphone abstinence.’ Should read: ‘However, while this may provide some evidence to confirm mood modification, our Bayes result suggests that more evidence is required to support any changes to mood before or after any period of smartphone abstinence.’ A sentence in the third paragraph of the discussion: ‘It is striking that drop-outs had higher SPAI scores on average’ Should read: ‘It is striking that drop-outs at session 3 had slightly higher SPAI scores.’

History

School

  • Sport, Exercise and Health Sciences

Published in

Addictive Behaviors

Volume

104

Publisher

Elsevier

Version

  • AM (Accepted Manuscript)

Rights holder

© Elsevier

Publisher statement

This paper was accepted for publication in the journal Addictive Behaviors and the definitive published version is available at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.106265

Publication date

2020-01-18

ISSN

0306-4603

eISSN

1873-6327

Language

  • en

Depositor

Dr Thom Wilcockson. Deposit date: 13 November 2020

Article number

106265

Usage metrics

    Loughborough Publications

    Categories

    No categories selected

    Exports

    RefWorks
    BibTeX
    Ref. manager
    Endnote
    DataCite
    NLM
    DC