[en] Early bilingualism acquired from home or community is generally considered to positively influence cognitive development. The purpose of the present study was to determine to what extent bilingualism acquired through a second-language immersion education has a similar effect. Participants included a total of 106 French-speaking 8-year-old children drawn from two language groups: 53 children enrolled in English immersion classes since the age of 5 (the immersion group) and 53 children enrolled in monolingual French-speaking classes (the monolingual group). The two groups were matched for verbal and nonverbal intelligence and SES. They were administered a battery of tasks assessing attentional and executive skills. The immersion group’s reaction times were significantly faster than those of the monolingual group on tasks assessing alerting, auditory selective attention, divided attention and mental flexibility, but not interference inhibition. These results show that, after only 3 years, a second-language immersion school experience also produces some of the cognitive benefits associated with early bilingualism.
Disciplines :
Theoretical & cognitive psychology
Author, co-author :
Nicolay, Anne-Catherine ; Université de Liège - ULiège > Département de Psychologie : cognition et comportement > Département de Psychologie : cognition et comportement
Poncelet, Martine ; Université de Liège - ULiège > Département de Psychologie : cognition et comportement > Neuropsychologie du langage et des apprentissages
Language :
English
Title :
Cognitive advantage in children enrolled in a second-language immersion elementary school program for 3 years
scite shows how a scientific paper has been cited by providing the context of the citation, a classification describing whether it supports, mentions, or contrasts the cited claim, and a label indicating in which section the citation was made.
Bibliography
Bialystok, E. (1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development, 70, 636- 644.
Bialystok, E. (2001). Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy, and cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Bialystok, E. (2005).Consequences of bilingualism for cognitive development. In Kroll & de Groot (eds.), pp. 417-432.
Bialystok, E. (2011). Coordination of executive functions in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 461-468.
Bialystok, E., & Barac, R. (2012). Emerging bilingualism: Dissociating advantages for metalinguistic awareness and executive control. Cognition, 122, 67-73.
Bialystok, E., & Codd, J. (1997). Cardinal limits: Evidence from language awareness and bilingualism for developing concepts of number. Cognitive Development, 12, 85-106.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Grady, C., Chau, W., Ishii, R., Gunji, A., & Pantev, C. (2005a). Effect of bilingualism on cognitive control in the Simon task: Evidence from MEG. NeuroImage, 24, 40-49.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19 (2), 290-303.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Luk, G. (2008). Lexical access in bilinguals: Effects of vocabulary size and executive control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21, 522-538.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Ryan, J. (2006). Executive control in a modified antisaccade task: Effects of aging and bilingualism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32 (6), 1341-1354.
Bialystok, E., &Majumder, S. (1998). The relationship between bilingualism and the development of cognitive processes in problem solving. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 69-85.
Bialystok, E., & Martin, M. M. (2004). Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: Evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. Developmental Science, 7 (3), 325- 339.
Bialystok, E., Martin, M. M., & Viswanathan, M. (2005b). Bilingualism across the lifespan: The rise and fall of inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 9, 103-119.
Bialystok, E., & Senman, L. (2004). Executive processes in appearance-reality tasks: The role of inhibition of attention and symbolic representation. Child Development, 75 (2), 562-569.
Brysbaert, M. (1998). Word recognition in bilinguals: Evidence against the existence of two separate lexicons. Psychologica Belgica, 38, 163-175.
Bunge, S. A., Dudukovic, N. M., Thomason, M. E., Vaidya, C. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002). Immature frontal lobe contributions to cognitive control in children: Evidence from fMRI. Neuron, 33, 301-311.
Butler, Y. G., & Hakuta, K. (2004). Bilingualism and second language acquisition. In T. K. Bhatia & W. C. Ritchie (eds.), The handbook of bilingualism, pp. 114-144. Oxford: Blackwell.
Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11 (2), 282-298.
Carr, T. H. (2004). A multilevel approach to selective attention: Monitoring environmental space, choosing stimuli for deep processing, and retrieving information from memory. In M. I. Posner (ed.), Cognitive neurosciences of attention, pp. 56-71. New York: Guilford Press.
Chevrie-Muller, C., Simon, A.-M., Le Normand, M. T., & Fournier, S. (2001). NEEL: Nouvelles Epreuves pour l'Examen du Langage [New Tests for Language Examination]. Paris: Editions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée.
Colzato, L. S., Bajo, M. T., van den Wildenberg, W., Paolieri, D., Nieuwenhuis, S., La Heij, W., & Hommel, B. (2008). How does bilingualism improve executive control? A comparison of active and reactive inhibition mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34 (2), 302-312.
Comblain, A., & Rondal, J.A. (2001). Apprendre les langues: où, quand, comment? [Learning languages: where, when, how?]. Liège: Mardaga.
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastián- Gallés, N. (2009). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don't. Cognition, 113, 135-149.
Costa, A., Hernández, M., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2008). Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106, 59-86.
Costa, A., & Santesteban, M. (2004). Lexical access in bilingual speech production: Evidence from language switching in highly proficient bilinguals and L2 learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 491-511.
Cummins, J. (1987). Bilingualism, language proficiency, and metalinguistic development. In P. Homel, M. Palij & D. Aaronson (eds.), Childhood bilingualism: Aspects of linguistic, cognitive and social development, pp. 57-73. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). PPVT-R: Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test - Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., Whetton, C., & Pintilie, D. (1982). BPVT: British Picture Vocabulary Test. Windsor: Nelson.
Dunn, L. M., Thériault-Whalen, C. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1993). EVIP: Echelle de Vocabulaire en Images Peabody [French adaptation of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test - Revised]. Richmond Hill, Canada: Psycan.
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 143-149.
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 340-347.
Gardner, F. M. (2000). The Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test (3rd edition). Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications.
Gollan, T. H., & Kroll, J. F. (2001). Bilingual lexical access. In B. Rapp (ed.), The handbook of cognitive neuropsychology: What deficits reveal about the human mind, pp. 321-345. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.
Green, C., & Bavelier, D. (2003). Action video game modifies visual selective attention. Nature, 423, 534-537.
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexicosemantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 67-81.
Inurritegui, S. (2009). Bilingualism and the development of cognitive control. Ph.D. dissertation, Katholieke Universiteit of Leuven.
Kovács, Á.M.,&Mehler, J. (2009). Cognitive gains in 7-monthold bilingual infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106 (16), 6556-6560.
Kroll, J. F., & de Groot, A. M. B. (eds.) (2005). Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Kroll, J. F., & Dijkstra, A. (2002). The bilingual lexicon. In R. Kaplan (ed.), Handbook of applied linguistics, pp. 301- 321. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Maguire, E. A., Gadian, D. G., Johnsrude, I. S., Good, C. D., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S., & Frith, C. D. (2000). Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 25/97 (9), 4414-4416.
Martin-Rhee, M. M., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11 (1), 81-93.
Meuter, R. F. I., & Allport, A. (1999). Bilingual language switching in naming: Asymmetrical costs of language selection. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 25-40.
Mezzacappa, E. (2004). Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: Developmental properties and sociodemographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child Development, 75 (5), 1373-1386.
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter,A.,&Wager,T.D. (2000).The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "frontal lobe" tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49-100.
Morton, J. B., & Harper, S. N. (2007). What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science, 10 (6), 719-726.
Morton, J. B., & Harper, S. N. (2009). Bilinguals show an advantage in cognitive control - The question is why. Developmental Science, 12 (4), 502-503.
Norman,D.A.,&Shallice, T. (1986).Attention to action:Willed and automatic control of behaviour. In R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz & D. Shapiro (eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation: Advances in research and theory, pp. 1- 18. New York: Plenum Press.
Paradis, M. (1984). Aphasie et traduction. META Translators' Journal, 29, 57-67.
Paradis, M. (2000). Aphasie chez les bilingues et les multilingues. In J. A. Rondal & X. Seron (eds.), Troubles du langage: Bases théoriques, diagnostic et rééducation, pp. 530-549. Liège: Mardaga.
Peal, E., & Lambert, W. (1962). The relation of bilingualism to intelligence. Psychological Monographs, 76 (546), 1-23.
Poulin-Dubois, D., Blaye, A., Coutya, J., & Bialystok, E. (2011). The effects of bilingualism on toddlers' executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108 (3), 567-579.
Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1998). Progressive Coloured Matrices. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press. Ricciardelli, L. A. (1992). Bilingualism and cognitive development in relation to threshold theory. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 21, 301-316.
Romaine, S. (1999). Bilingual language development. In M. Barret (ed.), The development of language, pp. 251-275. Hove: Psychology Press.
Schmidt, R. (2001). Attention. In P. Robinson (ed.), Cognition and second language instruction, pp. 3-32. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Segalowitz, N., & Hulstijn, J. (2005). Automaticity in bilingualism and second language learning. In Kroll & de Groot (eds.), pp. 371-388.
Tomlin, R.,&Villa,V. (1994).Attention in cognitive science and second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 16, 183-203.
Zelazo, P. D., Frye, D., & Rapus, T. (1996). An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them. Cognitive Development, 11, 37-63.
Zimmermann, P., Gondan, M., & Fimm, B. (2002). KiTAP: Test for Attentional Performance in Children. Herzogenrath: Psytest.
Similar publications
Sorry the service is unavailable at the moment. Please try again later.
This website uses cookies to improve user experience. Read more
Save & Close
Accept all
Decline all
Show detailsHide details
Cookie declaration
About cookies
Strictly necessary
Performance
Strictly necessary cookies allow core website functionality such as user login and account management. The website cannot be used properly without strictly necessary cookies.
This cookie is used by Cookie-Script.com service to remember visitor cookie consent preferences. It is necessary for Cookie-Script.com cookie banner to work properly.
Performance cookies are used to see how visitors use the website, eg. analytics cookies. Those cookies cannot be used to directly identify a certain visitor.
Used to store the attribution information, the referrer initially used to visit the website
Cookies are small text files that are placed on your computer by websites that you visit. Websites use cookies to help users navigate efficiently and perform certain functions. Cookies that are required for the website to operate properly are allowed to be set without your permission. All other cookies need to be approved before they can be set in the browser.
You can change your consent to cookie usage at any time on our Privacy Policy page.